How does the resource availability
hypothesis hold up with common garden snails (Helix aspersa) feeding on
winter wheat (Triticum aestivum)?
Chris Gibson
200035467
Project supervisor: Dr. David Pilbeam
Abstract
This
study will test whether the resource availability hypothesis is upheld in
feeding trials between winter wheat (Triticum
aestivum) and
common snails (Helix aspersa). The hypothesis predicts a positive trend
between growth rate and grazing. In feeding trials at the University Experimental
Gardens this was proved
to be upheld with an increase in final weight of the high nutrient treatment
plants supporting previous studies. The increase in mass is believed to be a
result of increased tillering or shoot growth stimulated by grazing. Snail
dispersal based on previous nutrition regimes was also tested. It was found
that prior nutrition strongly influences snail dispersal time. Tests on snail
pot choice (‘high’ or ‘low’ nutrient treatment plants) were however inconclusive.
It is believed that this is a result of the ‘low’ nutrient treatment not being
low enough to cause a significant difference in plant growth.
Introduction
What is the Resource Availability
Hypothesis?
The resource
availability hypothesis of antiherbivore defense was originally devised in
order to explain the variation in the levels of herbivory of plant species in
diverse ecosystems. It was based upon an original hypothesis that mature leaves
of slow growing species adapted to resource limited environments were less
preferred as a food source than mature leaves of rapid growing species adapted
to more productive and resources rich habitats. The first time it was tested
was in a study of nine woody South African plant species and kudu (Tragelaphus
strepsiceros) and impala (Aepyceros melampus) preferences for those
species (Bryant et al. 1989). The preferences for the plants were
previously known what wasn’t known was the inherent growth rate of the plants,
plants which were either palatable or unpalatable species. The growth rates of
the nine plants common in the study sites was determined from glass house
experiments. The comparison of predicted growth rates based upon kudu and
impala preferences with the actual growth rates obtained was use to uphold or
disprove the hypothesis. A positive correlation between predicted and measured
upheld the resource availability of antihervibore defense whereas negative or
lack of a correlation disproved the hypothesis. All results with the exception of
Grewia flavescens (which grew faster than other related species, however
is probably adapted to fertile soil) fit the hypothesis, which could be used to
explain why large African herbivores feed less on woody vegetation in the
unpalatable woodland and more on the palatable woodland.
This was backed by
other studies, which showed that large African herbivores feed less on
vegetation that is found on infertile soil than fertile soil vegetation. It is
suggested that the relationship between soil fertility, vegetation and large
herbivore foraging is due to infertile soil plants being poor food as it has a
low ratio of nutrients to fibre and chemical defenses (Bell , 1982).
Elberse et al.
(2003) conducted a series of experiments on nine wild barley (Hordeum spontaneum)
populations and their susceptibility to greenbug (Schizaphis graminum).
The study was set up to test the relationship between potential relative growth
rate and the barley’s susceptibility to the bug under two different nutrient
conditions and in light of the resource availability hypothesis (RAH). The RAH
predicted a positive correlation between potential relative growth rate and
susceptibility (in line with Bryant et al.’s work), results however
showed no correlation with high nutrient plants having on average 22.8 nymphs
per plant and low nutrient plants 24.3. As the RAH expects there to be some
trade off of growth rate for plant defense and as this was not present the RAH
hypothesis was, in this instance, discounted as effecting the relationship
between wild barley and greenbug. However previous studies have shown both a
positive (Gruber and Dixon 1988) and negative effect (Salas et al. 1990)
of nutrient levels on aphid performance if not specifically plant
susceptibility.
The difference between
the Bryant et al. and Elberse et
al. examples are that aphids and impalas feed differently on plant matter.
Aphids feed on the sap of a plant, which has an effect on the plant as a whole,
whereas animals and insects that graze only affect leaves or stems. This can be
shown with another example involving Triphabda beetles feeding on
goldenrods (Solidago spp.)(Brown 1994). In this study the resource
availability hypothesis was upheld as grazing increased relative growth rate
and as a direct consequence increased above ground biomass.
Diverse usage
As
a scientific hypothesis resource availability has a diverse usage in scientific
literature. Papers have been published treating resource availability like an
economic model (Neumayer 2000) or using an economic analogy in order to explain
trends seen in experiments (Bloom et al. 1985). The flexibility of the
hypothesis has allowed it to be widely used in research studies. One piece of
work involved manipulating resource levels and its effect on the Argentine ant
(Linepithema humile). In this
study the effect of altering levels of protein in different colonies diets and
its impact on pupae was investigated (Aron et al. 2001). The ants
were provided with high, intermediate or no extra protein in their diets, it
was shown that protein increases the number of sexual pupae produced. The
increase in protein meant females were more likely to be queens than workers
and the total proportion of males within the population was higher. The
un-supplemented nests showed that protein availability influenced the
proportion of a brood that is culled. The culling showed a direct relationship
to protein availability, the lower the level of protein the more sexual pupae
culled.
Andersson et al. (2005)
looked at differing resource levels (mircoinvertebrate and zooplankton
quantity) on Arctic char (Salvelinus aplinus). A series of pond and lake based experiments were carried out with
results showing that differences in the resource levels caused both
morphological and behavioural changes in the char. The fish that were in the
complex environment (high levels of both resources) had a higher dependency
upon the zooplankton than their simple environment counterparts. The complex
environment fish were subsequently morphologically dissimilar to the simple
environment fish as a direct response to this preference.
Byers (2000) showed that an increased level of resource availability
decreased the dispersal rate for larger (adult) esturine snails. Further
experiments showed that smaller snails had a higher dispersal rate regardless
of the resource levels present. This supported other studies, which had shown
less competition amongst the young smaller snails. One possible hypothesis for
these results was suggested as being some form of genetic behaviour present
when the snails are young. The behaviour is thought to cause younger snails to
disperse more readily in order to avoid competition.
Nutrient availability
and its effects
A review article by Aerts and Chapin III (2000) spoke about the effect
of increasing a plant’s need for nutrients and this impacting upon the plants
capacity to absorb the nutrient and how this capacity is specific for that
nutrient which most limits a plants growth. For example if a plant requires
more N then the need to uptake N will decrease the ability to absorb other
nutrients that do not limit the plants growth. At conditions of low nutrient
supply the plants ability for uptake will be at its maximum. It will also
reduce nutrient leakage at the roots thus allowing it to acquire nutrients at
lower external concentrations (Kronzucker et al. 1997).
Trials ran by Wilson and Tilman (1991) in a 30-year-old field. The
experiments concentrated on the effects of fertilization and disturbance on the
plant communities. The experiments were conducted on 104 5x5m plots separated
by a 2m corridor of untreated vegetation. Tilman had already demonstrated that
nitrogen had the greatest limitation on plant growth within that community in
which dominant species included Schizachyrium scoparium, Rumex acetosella, Lespedeza capitata,
Solidago nemoralis,
mosses and lichens. As it was the greatest limiting factor N was chosen to
represent fertilization within the experiments. There were four treatments: no
added nitrogen and highest rate of disturbance, no nitrogen and no disturbance,
highest added nitrogen and no disturbance and finally highest rate of nitrogen
and disturbance. It was shown that increasing the levels of nitrogen increased
the total community biomass. The species richness within the plots also
increased with nitrogen supply, this time however only in undisturbed plots. In
both the undisturbed and disturbed plots vegetation height increased as light
penetration decreased. This was coupled with a decrease in root: shoot ratios
with the added nitrogen. Leaf allocation decreased with disturbance whereas
flowering allocation actually increased despite stem allocation being
unaffected by disturbance levels. Similar results were found in yellow nutsedge
(Shibuya et al. 2004) where increasing nutrient
supply led to increasing plant productivity and associated traits of this, i.e.
flowering allocation. Plant productivity could also be seen as being a form of
competition between plants, where increased reproductive success is the outcome
of successfully out competing neighbouring plants. This has been backed up by
competition field studies where manipulation of resources was shown to affect
competition intensity (Davis and Pelsor 2001).
A different set of experiments by Wilson and Tilman (1993) this time on
a native perennial grass Schazchyrium scoparium only and using a similar method to their previous experiment with
disturbance equaling annual tilling which removed all vegetation in each of the
5x5m plots. There were two treatments of N simply non-added of 17gm-2yr-1.
They were able to show that above ground competition was greatest in plots with
the lowest light penetration. Lower nitrogen availability increased belowground
competition and decreased significantly with increasing nitrogen availability.
Grazing, plant defence
and resource availability
Grazing by herbivores reduces plant fitness. Increasing any form of
plant defence either inducible of constitutively expressed is therefore
expected to be selected for. Induced responses to herbivory are likened to
immune responses in that they can reduce the performance or preference of
herbivores. Adaptations made by the plants are assumed to be beneficial to
them. There is however a lack of experimental evidence demonstrating benefits.
There is evidence showing that following grazing plants increase their levels
of chemical, physical and biotic defences in many species. Some work has been
attempted to prove that these defences increase fitness by reducing herbivory
(Agrawal 1998) using wild radishes and testing 3 conditions; induced plants,
leaf damage controls and overall controls. Early season flower number was
measured as a correlation of male fitness. The induced experiment was conducted
early in the year and then the plants were left to be grazed by natural
herbivores. The induced plants showed a greater resistance too herbivory than
the control and leaf damage control plants with female reproductive fitness of
the plants being recorded as being 60% higher. This protection was not species
specific, with all small grazing herbivores and aphids affected. This is an example of defence plasticity;
plastic defences are when a single genotype can produce different phenotypes
depending upon the environment. Plastic responses are favoured by selection if
plants can respond appropriately to reliable information in their environments.
The other side of this is that if a plant is to act of inaccurate in formation
it would actually lower plant fitness as additional resources are being diverted
to less essential areas of the plant (Karban et al. 1999).
Transgenerational effect is another defence strategy. This is also
known as maternally induced defence. In this non-lethally grazed plants produce
defences against herbivores but will also produce offspring that are better
defended when compared with plants produced by non-threatened parents. This is
a form of phenotypic plasticity which works across generations as well as
within individuals within the original generation. Endowing offspring with defences
against future grazing which the parent plant has already dealt with will
increase the lifetime reproductive success of the second generation. This
occurs due to altering of the offspring’s phenotype. Work has been done on wild
radishes (Raphanus
raphanistrum)
taking them and exposing them to damage by a specialist caterpillar (Pieris rapae) induced a ten fold increase in
indole glucosinolates (mustard oil glycosides) as well as a 30% increase in
density of setose trichomes on newly formed leaves of the damaged plants when
compared with control’s (Agrawal et al. 1999).
In these tests there were control plants and plants that had 50% of each leaf
consumed by a caged caterpillar. In the experiments where the caterpillar
grazing occurred hydroxylated glucosinolates increased in concentration whereas
other classes of glucosinolates decreased. The caterpillars feeding on damaged
plant seedlings gained 20% less weight than those which were fed on the
seedlings of undamaged plants. This discrepancy was not explained by seed mass
variation of investment in primary metabolites. Seeds from these plants did not
differ in nitrogen of carbon content when compared with seeds from undamaged
plants as so investment in seedling defences was concluded to be the reason.
Another defence strategy which links in with this is the optimal
defence theory. This expects there to be a negative relationship between growth
and defence and so is similar to the RAH. This has been demonstrated in tomato
plants (Wilkens et
al. 1996). In
these tests the effect of resource availability on intraspecific and
within-plant allocation of soluble phenolics (rutin and chlorogenic acid) was
investigated. By measuring mass as well as physical and cellular attributes of
the plants the effects of resource availability on growth was also measured.
The experiments showed that plants grown in low resources contained low levels
of soluble phenolics and low plant mass. Plants grown in the intermediate
solution showed high phenolics but had inhibited growth. Those plants grown at
high levels of nutrients however had high growth but not higher phenolic
concentrations. The differences in phenolic concentrations were large enough to
have potential consequences for the insect herbivores feeding on the tomato
plants. This would have a knock-on effect of increasing the grazing on the
tomato plants grown with lower nutrient supply. Display of this is expected to
depend on the species of plant being tested, i.e. not every plant will display
chemical defences like this some may simply decrease their attractiveness to
herbivores (woody stems etc) as a means of defence.
Leaf lifetime can also be determined by resource availability as it
affects the relative advantages of defences with different turnover rates (Coley
et al. 1985). In fact the growth of
leaves has been shown to be strongly linked to bother resource availability and
plant defence strategies. Kurokawa et al.
(2004) used dark house experiments to show that a species which traditionally
grows in the shade (Eusideroxylon zwageri) has slow growth and in order to prolong lifespan devotes 35% of all
production to defensive substances (condensed tannins and lignin’s). This is
inline with what the resource availability hypothesis would predict for a slow
growing plant. This strategy is advantageous for survival in dark conditions as
these lignin’s and tannins help to prolong the lifespan of its leaves and stems
(tannins leaves and lignin’s stems). The growth differentiation balance is
another theory, which is different to resource availability and is used in
explaining the secondary metabolism and structural reinforcement that are physiologically
constrained in dividing and enlarging cells. This diverts resources away from
production of new leaf area (Herms and Mattson 1992).
The aims of the current study
This study will test whether the RAH is upheld in feeding trials
between winter wheat (Triticum
aestivum) and
common snails (Helix aspersa). In order to test this wheat will be grown
in two nutrient treatments a ‘high’ nutrient treatment and a ‘low’ nutrient
treatment.
RAH predicts that as the plants in the ‘high’ solution will have a greater RGR
and as wheat is a rapid growing species they will be more highly grazed upon
than the plants grown in the ‘low’ nutrient solution. Another aim will be to
look at snail dispersal and choice. If as previously stated an increase in
resource level increases snail dispersal rate then would treating the snails to
different diets prior to the trials affect this? For example starving the
snails for 24hours prior to trial is predicted to decrease dispersal rate (or
time till first movement) when compared to them being fed on wheat or fresh
vegetation. The difference between the fresh and wheat prior feeding treatment
however is uncertain. Also uncertain is the snail’s final choice (‘high’ or
‘low’) during the preference trials. While grazing on the high nutrient plants
is expected to be higher than on the low nutrient plants it is unknown as to
whether prior feeding will affect the preference trial results. This will also
be tested.
Materials and Methods
Snails and Wheat
Fourteen common garden snails of
various sizes were collected from the School of Biology Experimental Gardens.
These snails were kept in a tank that was aerated but had air holes too small
for the snails to escape through. They were regularly fed and misted in order
to ensure their survival during the course of this investigation.
Two crops of winter wheat were sown during the course of the experiment.
The first batch was sown on 23/09/05
and was used in the first free preference trials. The second batch of wheat,
sown on 8/11/05 ,
was used for the final four preference trials as well as the feeding
experiment. The wheat was all grown under glasshouse conditions and fed either
a ‘high’ nutrient solution of a ‘low’ nutrient solution depending upon which
treatment was being applied to it. The high nutrient solution was made up of;
KH2PO4 - 0.25mM, K2SO4 – 0.5mM,
MgSO4 – 0.5mM, Ca(NO3)2 – 1.0mM, IronEDTA –
0.04mM and micronutrients (H3BO3 – 0.0153mM, MnCl2 –
0.0005mM, ZnCl2 – 0.0005mM, CuCl2 – 0.0005mM, Na2MoO4
– 0.0024mM). The low nutrient solution was made from; ; KH2PO4
- 0.0625mM, K2SO4 – 0.125mM, MgSO4 – 0.125mM,
Ca(NO3)2 – 0.25mM, IronEDTA – 0.01mM and micronutrients
(H3BO3 – 0.003825mM, MnCl2 – 0.00125mM, ZnCl2
– 0.000125mM, CuCl2 – 0.000125mM, Na2MoO4
– 0.0006mM). The wheat was grown in sand in 95mm diameter and 90mm tall pots
and fed from the bottom up by supplying nutrient solution in a saucer 130mm
diameter and 25mm deep. To allow for individual growth each saucer was filled
up to the top and constantly topped up.
Preference trials
The preference trials tested the null hypothesis of the predictions
mentioned earlier. The wheat was placed in a preference board (figures 1 and
2). This board allowed the snails to move to any of the plants in the test
area. Testing occurred on individual snails and each trial lasted for twenty
minutes. This was determined after a mock trial was run using 10 snails at a
time (results not shown). The snails interacted with each other and as such
their movement was not felt to be a reflection of their own choice. The
twenty-minute time limit was decided as after twenty minutes the snails had
either chosen a pot of had stopped moving altogether. For each trial the
movement of the snail was recorded. The snail’s response was logged as either
staying in the middle of the board (or moving back to it), movement to a high
nutrient plant or movement to a low nutrient treatment plant. The snails
themselves were either fed wheat, fresh vegetation (randomly selected from the
gardens) or starved for 24hours prior to the trial commencing. For each
treatment 10 separate snails were recorded as repeats. All snails were placed
in the middle of the trial board and their original positioning was random. To
keep the snails on the board a ring of salt on top of Vaseline was applied to
the outside edge.
Figure 1,
dimensions of the trial board in mm.
Figure
2, the set up of the trial board. The ring of salt around the outside kept the
snails within the trial area, the saucers under the pots were the means by
which the plants could be watered and the plants themselves were accessible to
the snails during the entire course of the experiment.
Feeding trial
The feeding trial
ran for four days and three nights. All fourteen snails were involved and their
positions were logged daily. The snails were not touched during the trial and
were left on the board. This was designed to test the null hypothesis based on
the predictions. As well as the salt ring a second salt ring was set up on the
bench to prevent any snails escaping. As well as the trial board the where the
snails were allowed to graze a replicate trial board on which no snails were
placed was set p to allow comparisons with control plants that were grown in a
board but were not grazed. Comparisons were based on above ground dry weight of
individual plants.
Statistical analysis
Normally distributed data was analyzed
using ANOVA and T-tests. Non-parametric data was analyzed using a
Kruskal-Wallis test.
Results
Firstly a series of experiments were conducted
to explore whether or not the snails would eat winter wheat. An observation
experiment and a prolonged feeding experiment were set up. In the feeding
experiment the snails were left for five days with only wheat shoots for food.
The difference between the original mass of wheat and the final mass was
41.06g. This was compared with a similar mass of wheat left un-grazed for the
same period of time to determine mass difference due to water loss and plant
growth. The wheat from the drying experiment differed by 11.58g from its
original mass. The extra difference in mass in the feeding experiment was
accredited to grazing by the snails. In the observation experiment a single
snail was kept in captivity for a day with a diet of wheat. The snail was then
observed eating the wheat fed to it.
All results
gathered from the preference trials and the feeding experiment have been tested
for normality using Lillefors test for normality (not shown). The preference
trial results were found to be non-parametric whereas the feeding trial results
are normally distributed.
Preference
Trials
The snails had
three choices of response when placed upon the trial board. Firstly they could
stay in the middle (either sitting stationary or moving in circles) or disperse
to either a plant grown on a high nutrient solution or a plant grown on a low
nutrient solution. To remove any possibility of directional bias the pots were
moved around to different positions in the board between trials. Records of
which pots the snails moved towards (not shown) and finished at were also
recorded for several of the trials (figure 3). Based on these data movement of
the snails did not appear to be linked to any directional bias.
The data for time
spent in the middle of the trial board, which includes snails that did not
moved for the entire trial, (figure 4) showed that the treatment imposed upon
the snails before the trial (fed fresh vegetation, fed wheat, starved)
significantly affected the amount of time spent in the middle (Kruskal-Wallis,
p=0.026, N=60). Figure 4 shows snails fed wheat spent most time in the middle.
However figure 5 shows that on average feeding the snails wheat before a trial
caused a faster first movement than both the other treatments. This discrepancy
is a result of some snails fed wheat not moving for the entire trial. This
would have increased the time spent in the middle but not affected the time
taken until first movement as there was no first movement. There is a strong
relationship between treatment and first movement (Kruskal-Wallis, p=0.014,
N=54). The time spent at ‘higher’ and ‘lower’ pots however was shown to not
have a relationship with the snail treatments (charts 4 and 5) (Kruskal-Wallis,
p= 0.155 (high) and p=0.415 (low), N=60 (for both)).
Figure 3. Pots on which snails were present at the end of each trial.
There was no directional bias within the experimental set up which could have
affected snail choice.
Figure 4. Comparisons of the
time spent in the middle for each treatment. Fresh = fresh vegetation fed to
the snails, wheat= wheat fed to the snails, starved= snails were starved for
24hours prior to experiment. Snail treatment has an effect on the amount of
time the snails spent in the middle.
Figure 5. The time taken until the first move for each snail treatment.
Treatment type has a strong relationship with time taken until first movement
away from the middle.
Figure 6. The time spent at the plants grown on a high nutrient solution
for the snail treatment types. Analysis shows that treatment type did not
affect the time spent at these plants.
Figure 7. Time spent at the low nutrient treatment plants for the
different snail treatments. The treatments imposed on the snails were shown to
not affect the time spent at these plants.
Feeding Trial
The feeding trial
ran for four days. In this time the snails were left undisturbed and allowed to
graze and move at will. The positions of the snails were recorded daily (Table
1). A larger spread of low nutrient treatment plants were visited during the
trial. However on average these plants were not frequented by as many snails.
It is unclear whether snails stayed at certain pots or made changes in their
pot choice. Those not at a pot were either in between specific pots or had managed to find away off the
trial board.
|
Pot No.
|
||||||||
Day
|
Ha
|
Hb
|
Hc
|
Hd
|
La
|
Lb
|
Lc
|
Ld
|
Not at a
pot
|
1
|
0
|
0
|
2
|
4
|
1
|
1
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
2
|
0
|
0
|
2
|
3
|
0
|
0
|
1
|
5
|
3
|
3
|
0
|
0
|
2
|
6
|
0
|
0
|
0
|
2
|
4
|
4
|
0
|
0
|
2
|
4
|
0
|
0
|
0
|
1
|
7
|
Table 1.Positions of the sails recorded daily.
On day four the
pots were harvested of above ground biomass. This was then dried and
subsequently weighed. Comparisons of final masses for the grazed and non-grazed
plants (figures 8 and 9) showed that there was a significant difference in
weight between the plants that had been grazed upon and those that had not
(ANOVA, p=0.018, DF=1). Further analysis of these trends showed that
significant difference in mass between the grazed plants (T-test, p=0.038,
DF=6) whereas the difference between the un-grazed plants was not significant
(T-test, p=0.721, DF=6).
Figure 8. Mass between the grazed plants. The high nutrient treatment
plants were significantly heavier than the plants fed a lower nutrient
solution.
Figure 9. Mass of the non-grazed plants. The non-grazed plants were not
significantly dissimilar in final mass.
The final weights
of the grazed and un-grazed replicates (figures 10 and 11) showed that the low
nutrient solution plants were not significantly dissimilar in final mass
(T-test, p=0.504, DF=6). The high nutrient treatment plants however were
significantly different in final weight, with the grazed plants heavier than
their un-grazed counterparts (T-test, p=0.008, DF=6).
Figure 10. The final weights of the low nutrient treatment plants. There
is no significant difference in final mass.
Figure 11. Final dry mass of high nutrient plants. The differences are
significant.
Temperature and sunlight
The temperature
data for the growth glasshouse in which the wheat used in the feeding trial,
was grown in (figure 12) shows the wheat to have been grown at a consistent
temperature from germination to removal for use.
Figure 12. Temperature data for the growth greenhouse in November. 1=
8/11 the germination date, 2= 29/11 when the wheat was moved from the
glasshouse for use.
Charts 11 and 12
show November and December temperatures within the glasshouse used for the
preference trial and feeding experiment there was only one low point in
temperature at the end of November (no.4 on graph). Charts 13 and 14 show the
hours of daylight for the period that the experiments were being run. During
this time the shift in daylight hours was not enough to seriously effect plant
growth
Figure 13. Temperature data for the trial greenhouse in November. 1=trial
of experiment (used 10 snails fed on fresh vegetation, results not shown), 2=
first preference trial (starved snails), 3= second preference trial (fed fresh
vegetation), 4= third preference trial (starved), first trial to use second
wheat batch.
Figure 14. Temperature data for the trial greenhouse in December. 1=fifth
trial (fed on wheat), 2=sixth trial (starved), pot order was randomly altered,
3=seventh trial (fed fresh vegetation), start of feeding experiment, 4=end of
feeding trial and harvesting of wheat.
Figure 15. Day length for November.
Figure 16. Day length for December.
Discussion
The first thing to notice is that on the un-grazed board the ‘high’ and
the ‘low’ nutrient plants were insignificantly different in final dry mass
(means of high-1.086g and low-1.051g). This similarity in mass was most likely
a result of the low nutrient solution in fact being too high already and being
at the top of the plants growth curve (figure 17).
Preference
The time taken till first move was higher for the starved snails than
those fed prior to the preference trials (6.42 minutes compared with 3.93 and 3
minutes). This supports Byers (2000) findings on resource availability
affecting snail dispersal rates. However the effect of prior feeding on snail
pot choice was inconclusive. Either the snails randomly choose which plant they
first approach or they could not choose between the ‘high’ or ‘low’ nutrient
plants.
Figure 17. The low nutrient
solution (the arrow) shown to be a nutrient concentration which was close to
the optimal growth of the plant.
Feeding trials
The RAH was upheld
in that as predicted the plants grown on high nutrient solution were grazed on
more. This grazing corresponded to an increase in final dry mass when compared
to the other plants. The non-grazed plants showed no significant difference in
final dry mass this most likely due to the low nutrient solution being too high
a concentration. However that the masses of the grazed plants were
significantly different (high- 1.274g and low- 1.123g) and the un-grazed and
grazed ‘high’ plants were also significantly different the only reason for the
mass differences must be grazing. Grazers have been shown to increase mass in
plants before however (Brown 1994, Agrawal et al. 1999). What is unsure
is the specific reasons behind the increase in mass in this experiment. However
it has been documented that increase in grazing in grasses has increased above
ground biomass (Georgiadis et al. 1989) with an increase in tillering
and shoot growth as possible explanations for this.
Further work
Growth of the
plants in a lower concentration low nutrient solution (possibly twice as dilute
as the current concentration) and re-running of the preference trials would
hopefully both support findings from this project. It would be hoped that the
with this change the effect of prior nutrition on the snails preference might
show some clear result, if indeed there is a clear result to be had.
With the plants
grown at more distinct nutrient treatments the feeding trials should support
the findings here. What can be investigated is both tiller number before and
after grazing and shoot length before and after grazing. Hopefully this will
prove which of these is responsible for the increase in mass observed here.
Another alteration
could be too look at the effect of snail age (based on size) on the experiments
and by testing snails of similar sizes does this alter the outcomes of the
trials.
Conclusion
For the feeding
trial RAH was upheld showing that plants growing faster were grazed upon more
than slower growing plants. That those plants did no necessarily grow bigger
than other plants on offer to the snails before grazing supports this. The
increase in mass as a result of the grazing was most likely a result of extra
tillering or shoot growth stimulated by the grazing. Preference trial studies
showed that dispersal rate increased with prior feeding regimes although actually
distinction and choice showed a random pattern.
Acknowledgements
Without the advice
and aid of Mr. Martin Lappage and Miss Chloe Thompson at the University Experimental
Gardens and the advice,
experience and patience of Dr. David Pilbeam this project would never have been
possible. As such my thanks and gratitude goes out to them.
References
Bryant J.P.,
Kuropat P.J, Cooper S.M., Frisby K., and Owen-Smith N.. (1989) Resource
availability hypothesis of plant antiherbivore defence tested in a South
African savanna ecosystem
Nature 340:
227-230
Elberse I.A.M.,
Turin J.H.B., Wackers F.L., Van Damme J.M.M., Van Tienderen P.H. (2003) The
relationship between relative growth rate and susceptibility to aphids in wild
barely under different nutrient levels. Oecologia 137: 564-571
Gruber K. and Dixon AFG. (1988) The
effect of nutrient stress on development and reproduction in an aphid. Extomol.
Exp. Appl. 47:23-30
Salas ML, Corcuera
LJ, Argandona VH (1990) Effect of potassium nitrate on gramine content and
resistance of barley against the aphid Schizaphis
graminum. Phytochemistry 29:3789-3791
Brown D.G. (1994)
Beetle folivory increases resource availability and alters plant invasion in
monocultures of goldenrod. Ecology 75(6): 1673-1683
Neumayer E. Scarce
or Abundant? (2000) The Economics of Natural Resource Availability. Journal of
Economic Surveys 14(3): 307-335
Bloom A.J., Chapin
III F.S., Mooney H.A. (1985) Resource limitation in plants – an economic
analogy. Ann.Rev.Ecol.Syst. 16:363-392
Aron S., Keller L.
and Passera L.. (2001) Role of resource availability on sex, caste and
reproductive allocation ratios in the Argentine ant Linepithema humile Journal
of Animal Ecology 70:831-839
Andersson J.,
Bystrom P., Persson L. and De Roos A.M. (2005) Plastic resource polymorphism:
effects of resource availability on Arctic char (Salvelinus aplinus)
morphology. The Linnean Society of London ,
Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 85, 341-351
Byers J.E. (2000) Effects of body size and resource availability
on dispersal in a native and non-native esturine snail. Journal of Experimental
Marine Biology and Ecology 248:133-150
Aerts R. &
Chapin III F.S. (2000) The Mineral Nutrition of Wild Plants Revisited: A
Re-evaluation of Processes and Patterns. Advances in Ecological Research 30:2-70
Kronzucker H.J.,
Siddiqi. M.Y., and Glass, A.M. (1997) Conifer root discrimination against soil
nitrate and the ecology of forest succession. Nature 385: 59-61
Wilson S.D. and
Tilman D. (1991) Interactive effects of fertilization and disturbance on
community structure and resource availability in an old-field plant community.
Oecologia 88:61-71
Shibuya B.L.T.,
Yogo Y. and Hara T. (2004) Effects of ramet clipping and nutrient availability
on growth and biomass allocation of yellow nutsedge. Ecological Research 19:
603-612
Davis M.A. and
Pelsor M. (2001) Experimental support for a resource-based mechanistic model of
invisibility. Ecology Letters 4:
421-428.
Wilson S.D.,
Tilman D. (1993) Plant competition and resource availability in response to
disturbance and fertilization. Ecology 74(2): 599-611.
Agrawal A.A.
(1998) Induced Responces to Herbivory and Increased Plant Performance. Science 279:
1201-1202.
Karban R., Agrawal
A.A., Thaler J.S. and Adler L.S. (1999) Induced plant responses and information
content about risk of herbivory. Tree 14 (11): 443-447.
Agrawal A.A.,
Laforsch C. and Tollrian R. (1999) Transgenerational induction of defences in
animals and plants. Nature 401: 60-63.
Wilkens R.T.,
Spokere J.M. and Stamp N.E. (1996) Differential responces of growth and two
soluble phenolics of tomato to resource availability. Ecology 77(1):
247-258.
Coley P.D., Bryant
J.P., Chapin III F.S. (1985) Resource Availability and Plant Antiherbivore
Defence. Science 230 (4728): 895-899
Kurokawa H.,
Kitahashi Y., Koike T., Lai J., Nakashizuka T. (2004) Allocation to defense or
growth in dipterocarp forest seedlings in Borneo. Oecologia 140:261-270.
Herms D.A.,
Mattson W.J. (1992) The dilemma of plants: to grow or defend. Quarterly Review
of Biology 67 (3): 283-335.